When Public Policy Meets Elementary Biology

I have been thinking a lot about public policy, welfare, support of the indigent, and the growth of the underclass in industrial societies, mostly to prepare for this meeting. Public discussion and journalism routinely identify people as “right wing” or “left wing”. My reaction is that most public commentary—on both the right and the left—is hardly worth out attention.

Some trends that I find particularly distressing are outlined by Charles Murray and Robert Putnam in a youtube video which we discussed in a previous post. Both Murray and Putnam describe growing numbers of the underclass in this country with their failure of community and family organization. Single mothers are normative. Both speakers focus on white people: Murray explicitly restricts his recent book to ‘White America’ while Putnam’s new book is ethnographic in style about his own home town, again mostly white. Is there a way out of the trends they describe through social engineering?

Neither Murray nor Putnam have much in the way of policy suggestions. Murray identifies increasing isolation between the prosperous and the impoverished and the failure of the prosperous to advocate their own moral and social values to the to the poor. Putnam advocates a Soviet style system of public education in which teachers assume the duties and roles of parents, starting with early childhood education.

Politicians, journalists, and education advocates agonize a lot about issues of our social future but most of it is wordspeak and twaddle. Many politicians are shameless vote chasers with no principles, journalists seek sensation and scandal, and educators have had no real accomplishments in a century. What would be the outcome if social engineers understood evolution? Does biology have a contribution to make to the solution of do our social and economic problems?

The reasoning would go like this (straight from freshman biology): diploid organisms are shaped by evolution to generate copies of their DNA. In order to make these copies a diploid organism has to allocate energy and risk to competing demands of (a) growth and maintenance and (b) reproduction. Reproduction has two parts, mating and parenting. This allocation is the stuff of life history theory. The allocation problem is complicated by the presence of two sexes that are designed differently. This is especially so in mammals: internal gestation, mammary glands, and prolonged immaturity indicate of the commitment of females to bear the brunt of reproductive effort. Fish, for example, are not engineered in this way. In fish species where males mouth brood, mama fish is free to shed some eggs and abandon dad and the kids to continue her partying unimpeded.

Humans exhibit a diversity of strategy “choices” that are solutions to the allocation problem between mating and parenting. Males can devote most of their effort to mating effort, usually involving competition with other males. Male commitment to parenting effort is not common in mammals but there are familiar examples like beavers, coyotes, gibbons, and some humans. In the jargon the polar strategies of male mammals are called “cad” and “dad” strategies.

Females have a more restricted set of strategy choices because of their engineered commitment to parenting. At one extreme a human female can seek a dadly male who provides resources like food and protection to their joint offspring. At the other extreme, a human female can pay little or no attention to her mate choice, instead letting the guys work things out. In the jargon these female alternatives are called “coy” and “fast”.

You can find a more detailed account of this game between the human sexes works in a chapter of our book (that the editor discarded as “too academic”) on our website here. Briefly we are likely to find dad males/coy females in ecological situations where male labor and resources are critical for successful reproduction. Think of labor-intensive agriculture, European peasants and Asian farmers, as examples. In the United States in the past, “working class” meant stable mated pairs who together provisioned and cared for children. An archetype of working class in American television was Archie Bunker.

Social organization with cad males and fast females is found prominently among tropical gardeners where women provide most of the food for themselves and their children as well as for the men, who are often just parasites on the women. The euphemism in economics for these societies is “female farming systems”. These share many characteristics with our industrial “underclass” in which women have no ecological force pushing them into long term stable pair bonds.

Notice that in each of the above descriptions there are two hands clapping: in cad/fast social systems neither a coy female nor a dad male does very well while in dad/coy systems neither a fast females nor a cad male does very well. The two polar social types are deeply rooted in contemporary politics. The zany feminism of the 1980s (“a woman needs a man like a fish needs a bicycle”) precisely advocated the cad/fast setup. Our religious right with its chatter about “the natural family” and “stable marriages” and the like pushes hard for a dad/coy world.

Back to our our social engineers who know biology. They share a goal of a society in which dad males mate with coy females because children enjoy the care and security of a stable home and streets safe from gunfire. The new policy is simple: welfare payments are to be given only to males.

This policy would mimic, they think, the ecology of most dad/coy societies. How would this work out? In a new post we can imagine how the new policy can be modified when the engineers are given a sense of human decency and responsibility for human well being.

Part II to follow ……

Posted in Uncategorized | Tagged , , | 31 Comments

Breadth-first search

When I complain about some egregious piece of research,  particularly those that are in some sense cross-disciplinary, I often feel that that just knowing more would solve the problem. If Roland Fryer or Oded Galor understood genetics, they wouldn’t make these silly mistakes. If Qian and Nix understood genetics or American post-Civil War history, they would never have written that awful paper about massive passing.  Or if paleoanthropologists and population geneticists had learned about mammalian hybrids, they would have been open to the idea of Neanderthal introgression.

But that really amounts to a demand that people learn about five times as much in college and grad school as they actually do.  It’s not going to happen.  Or, perhaps, find a systematic and effective way of collaborating with people outside their discipline without having their heads shaved. That doesn’t sound too likely either.

 

Posted in Uncategorized | 41 Comments

Dogs and Men

Razib Khan talks about a new article that suggests that dogs were domesticated quite a long time ago, perhaps more than 35,000 years ago, well before the last glacial maximum.

We know that dogs have adapted to life with people, have changed in many ways.

I wonder how humans adapted to dogs.  If they were like modern pariah dogs, hanging around the village and eating garbage, doesn’t seem that they would have been that influential. But if used in hunting, they could have been very important, especially back in the Ice Age – and if they were that important, the partnership might have generated significant selective pressures in humans.

Parenthetically, there was an article a few years back that claimed that dogs had probably been domesticated for ~100,000 years, while most other estimates were around 14,000 years. Obviously there was a simple way to reconcile those two numbers.

 

 

Posted in Uncategorized | 42 Comments

One more time

One of our local error sources suggested that it would be impossible to rebuild technical civilization, once fallen. Now if every human were dead I’d agree, but in most other scenarios it wouldn’t be particularly difficult, assuming that the survivors were no more silly and fractious than people are today.  So assume a mild disaster, something like the effect of myxomatosis on the rabbits of Australia, or perhaps toe-to-toe nuclear combat with the Russkis – ~90%  casualties worldwide.

Describe the recovery process, why it’s feasible and in fact almost easy. Show your work.

There’s no reason that I should have to explain everything.

Posted in Uncategorized | 127 Comments

Measles and immunological amnesia

A new paper in Science , by Michael Mina et al,  strongly suggests that measles messes up your immunological defenses for two or three years. This is the likely explanation for the fact that measles inoculation causes much greater decreases in child morbidity and mortality than you’d expect from preventing the deaths directly due to measles infection. The thought is that measles whacks the cells that carry immunological memory, leaving the kid ripe for reinfections.  I think there can be a similar effect with anti-cancer chemotherapy.

If correct, this means that measles is much nastier than previously thought. It must have played a significant role in the demographic collapse of long-isolated peoples (such as the Amerindians). Its advent may have played a role in the population decrease associated with the decline of the Classical world.  Even though it is relatively new (having split off from rinderpest a couple of thousand years ago) strong selection for resistance may have  favored some fairly expensive genetic defenses (something like sickle-cell) in Eurasian populations.

We already know of quite a few complex side effects of infectious disease, such the different kind of immunosuppression we see with AIDs, Burkitt’s lymphoma hitting kids with severe Epstein-Barr infections followed by malaria, acute dengue fever that requires a previous infection by a different strain of dengue, etc: there may well be other important interactions and side effects, news of which has not yet come to Harvard.

Posted in Uncategorized | Tagged , | 14 Comments

Eugenics, Ready or Not

There is an excellent essay about eugenics, genetic manipulation, and technology by Frank Salter here in the Australian webzine Quadrant and reprinted here. The essay is nearly free of the incessant hand wringing that pervades most journalism about the topic. Instead the treatment is thorough, wide ranging, and sensible—journalism for intelligent adults. Science writers will blush with shame as they read this essay.

The links above are to part one of the article: part two is promised to appear “shortly”.

Posted in Uncategorized | Tagged , , | 73 Comments

SNP confusion

A single nucleotide polymorphism is a nucleotide that is variable – not everyone has the same one at that site.  To be more precise, it’s one that has significant variation – at least a few percent of people (in the population you’re studying) have something other than the most common nucleotide at that site.  Generally, the most common nucleotide is the ancestral one.

You see such a variable site every few hundred nucleotides.  Checking out the SNPs is less informative than sequencing the whole genome, but it’s much easier.

Most of these  SNPs are in neutral parts of the genome and don’t do anything, one way or the other. If you’re investigating ancient population splits and such, this is useful, easier to analyze: neutral means unloaded dice, adaptive evolution means dice loaded in unpredictable ways.

In order to pick a set of SNPs, you look at members of some population and see which sites in the genome are sufficiently variable.  They may still be good SNPs in another population, as  long as that population is sufficiently similar – that is, shares most of the same drift history.

On the other hand, if the two populations have been separated for a long, long time,  they won’t have the same SNPS, at least not entirely: sites that are variable in population 1, the population  you used to pick your SNPS, may be fixed in the second population, while sites that are essentially fixed in the first population may be SNPs in population 2.  The long the separation, the more this happens.

Some years ago, some friends of mine looked at how ancestral (on average) SNPs were in different populations.

chimps

The average degree of ancestralness was almost exactly the same in Eurasian populations, but was noticeably higher in typical farming populations of sub-Saharan Africa such as the Yoruba, and was higher still in African hunter-gatherers such as the Pygmies and Bushmen.

We found this puzzling, and it took a while to figure it out. The root cause turned out to be  that the drift histories of Eurasians were pretty much all the same, but the drift history of the Yoruba was different enough that they had somewhat different SNPs than Eurasians, while the Bushmen & Pygmies had split off even earlier and had an even more different drift history.

This point is technical, even boring once you figure out the answer, but add some misunderstanding&ideology  and the story gets hilarious.

Now this following story I mostly got second hand, so take it with a grain of salt. But I think it’s basically accurate.

You see, somewhat earlier, people working with the same datasets noticed the same phenomenon, and instead of puzzling through it, they panicked.  They thought that those higher levels of ancestrality in Africans meant that Africans were genetically closer to chimpanzees  than other humans – which is not the case.  One of them came up with the memorable phrase ‘chimp index‘ for average ancestrality.

Around the same time, there was a lot of odd activity at NIH, and I strongly suspect that activity was connected to the chimp index crisis. Even at the time, before we looked at this problem, I thought that something strange was going on. They called a special meeting, where those receiving NIH grants in human genetics were told to be careful to make sure that reporters didn’t get the wrong idea about genetic differences between populations.  Marc Feldman proposed organizing ‘flying squads’ to rush out and counteract such misunderstandings – which inspired me to design a shoulder patch for those flying squads:

REDgreenBLACK-3

Eventually someone who knew some theory must have figured it out and explained it, which would have calmed things down.  But for a while, the powers that be at NIH must have been shitting in their pants.  How awful.

Posted in Uncategorized | Tagged | 33 Comments